Skip to content
masistvucutcu

masistvucutcu

masistvucutcu

  • Home
  • Real estate
  • Transport
  • Health care
  • Human activities
  • Toggle search form

Our Occasional Inability to Notice What’s in Front of Us

Posted on August 4, 2022 By admin No Comments on Our Occasional Inability to Notice What’s in Front of Us

Source: Philippspence/Pixabay

There are limits to humans’ visual attention. Some people, in some circumstances, can look at a scene and miss something right in front of them. This oversight can sometimes have serious consequences, such as when a driver doesn’t see a cyclist or a radiologist misses a tumor.

The award-winning study on this topic by Simons and Chabris (1999) led to a best-selling book called The Invisible Gorilla: How Our Intuitions Deceive Us (Chabris and Simons, 2009). The “invisible gorilla” refers to the 1999 result as a case of “inattentional blindness” in which some participants didn’t recall seeing someone in a gorilla suit right in front of them while they focused on the passing of basketballs.

Exaggerated Reports

Although the book reported that “roughly half” of the participants did recall the gorilla, many writers and instructors have since exaggerated the effect. It’s common to read or hear that people “frequently” or “often” missed the gorilla, that they “tend[ed] not to notice,” or even that “most people” missed it.

In my book about biases (Stalder, 2018), I referenced this language and further revealed that it was less than half—43 percent—missed the gorilla in a real-life context. There were multiple conditions in the original study, only some of which represented real-life viewing. In the few other conditions, the gorilla was “partially transparent” (to mimic an earlier procedure of superimposing different displays). When the gorilla was barely visible in this condition, about 70 percent missed it. It was only by combining the transparent and real-life conditions that the proportion who missed it rose to half. Even 43 percent was an amazing result, but it was not the norm.

Numerous educational videos and reports share real-life images and not the barely visible transparent gorilla (to see the transparent condition, you can scroll to p.1067 in the original article).

My book’s general point regarded a common bias to overgeneralize (which underlies stereotyping). The exaggerated reporting of the gorilla effect was just one of multiple high-profile examples. My advice was to be wary of vague or overinclusive nonstatistical language like “frequently” or “most people” in reading about psychological effects.

An Updated Review: “Normal Blindness”

In recent years, public attention to the gorilla effect has died down. But a new study by Wolfe and colleagues (July 21, 2022) has brought the not-so-invisible gorilla back into the news (Carroll, 2022; Pearson, 2022). Wolfe and colleagues reviewed the gorilla effect and more recent results, proposed a single framework to cover all such effects, and coined a new name for our occasional oversight: “normal blindness.”

The authors wrote that “humans routinely miss important information that is “right in front of our eyes,” from overlooking typos in a paper to failing to see a cyclist in an intersection.” Although it’s true that humans have visual limitations, terms like “normal” and “routinely” raise my previous question about overgeneralizing.

Wolfe and colleagues first reviewed the classic invisible-gorilla study. They reported that “about 50 percent” missed the gorilla, again overlooking that the percentage was actually lower in real-life conditions. In another initial example, people looked for a T in a series of L’s and missed the T 5-10 percent of the time, which does not really rise to a “routine” level.

More important than proofreading are cases of driver error or medical misdiagnosis. Wolfe and colleagues cited studies on driving accidents in which a cyclist was not seen in time. However, they did not provide the overall proportion of times that cyclists were missed (my limited look at the research suggests it’s a minority of the time). Some of those studies established that drivers who don’t expect to see a cyclist or are otherwise distracted are more likely not to see the cyclist. Make sense.

hkgoldstein0/Pixabay

Source: hkgoldstein0/Pixabay

Similarly, radiologists looking for one type of anomaly are more likely to miss another. Among multiple cited studies, Williams and colleagues (2021) reported that “when their attention was focused on searching for lung nodules, 66 percent of radiologists did not detect breast cancer and 30 percent did not detect lymphadenopathy.” These error rates dropped to 3 and 10 percent, respectively, when “searching for a broader range of abnormalities.”

Wolfe and colleagues also cited Wolfe’s earlier work (2005), which established that the more rare or infrequent the target item is, the more often observers miss it. Overall, common items were missed less than 10 percent of the time, whereas the rarest items were missed about 35 percent of the time (with one condition exceeding 50 percent).

Acknowledgments

To their credit, Wolfe and colleagues (2022) acknowledged a recent study that suggested that part of why people don’t report having seen something right in front of them is a memory failure and not that they didn’t originally notice it (Robbins et al. al., 2019). Wolfe and colleagues also identified the random nature of such errors in concluding that “an item that one observer at one time misses will be seen at a different time or by a different observer.” Thus, a primary piece of advice was to seek “a second pair of eyes.”

In Sum

People have visual limitations. And when drivers’ or radiologists’ attentional mistakes result in fatalities, that is tragic. Identifying or reminding us about the conditions under which these oversights are more likely is thus an important part of Wolfe and colleagues’ work. It can save lives.

However, these goals can be achieved without overstating the typically-less-than-50-percent frequency of the errors (although I acknowledge subjectivity in terms like “frequently” and “routinely”). The classic invisible-gorilla study, particularly, is well known and still commonly cited in science classrooms. It seems important to highlight that this attentional mistake was not the norm while still emphasizing its dangers in certain contexts. The broader benefits of not overgeneralizing include reduced stereotyping, whether about particular groups or humans in general.

Health care

Post navigation

Previous Post: Putting Collective Momentum Into Behavior Change
Next Post: Are Your Prepared for Your Psychiatric Consultation?

Leave a Reply Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Archives

  • August 2022
  • July 2022
  • June 2022
  • May 2022

Categories

  • Health care
  • Human activities
  • Real estate
  • Transport

Recent Posts

  • It is possible to understand Scott Morrison? An expert has a shot
  • Why ‘Dealing With People’ Has Become Our Biggest Struggle
  • First Human US Implant: Synchron Brain-Computer Interface
  • Can the Superiority Illusion Benefit Collectors?
  • The Managed Consumer | Psychology Today

Recent Comments

No comments to show.
  • About us
  • Contact us
  • DMCA
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and conditions

Copyright © 2022 masistvucutcu.

Powered by PressBook WordPress theme